Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 13 de 13
Filtrar
Mais filtros










Intervalo de ano de publicação
1.
Int. j. morphol ; 41(5): 1461-1466, oct. 2023. tab, graf
Artigo em Inglês | LILACS | ID: biblio-1521034

RESUMO

SUMMARY: Measurements of the upper strait of the pelvis can be calculated using the Anterior Pelvic Index. The objective of the study was to determine the external validity and cut-off point of the API, to classify narrow pelvises from normal ones. We selected 214 women from 15 to 55 years old, 171 had vaginal delivery and 43 by caesarean section by feto-pelvic disproportion (FPD) of maternal origin, in whom the API was calculated, of which its mean difference was established with an alpha error of <0.05. Maximum values of sensitivity and specificity, ROC curve and Youden index were determined. The student's t gave a p-value =0.000 of the mean difference between the women who had vaginal delivery and those who had cesarean section by FPD of maternal origin; the value of the area under the ROC curve was 0.758 (CI 95% 0.695 - 0.814) with a p-value=0.0001. Maximum sensitivity was 74.42 % (CI 95%: 58.8 % to 86.5 %) and maximum specificity was 73.10 % (CI 95%: 65.8 % to 79.6 %), produced a Youden index of 0.475 (CI 95% 0.283 - 0.590) which is associated with the 15.44 (CI 95% 14.19 - 15.83) of the API scale. The API is a good tool for predicting women with suspected narrow pelvis and allows its classification into three types of pelvises: an API value of more than 15.83 would indicate pelvis suitable for vaginal delivery; an API value between 14.19 and 15.83 would be suspected of pelvic narrowness; an API value less than 14.19 would confirm a narrow pelvis.


Las medidas del estrecho superior de la pelvis pueden calcularse mediante el Índice Pelviano Anterior. El objetivo del estudio fue determinar la validez externa y el punto de corte del API, para clasificar pelvis estrechas de las normales. Seleccionamos 214 mujeres de 15 a 55 años, 171 tuvieron parto vaginal y 43 mediante cesárea por DFP de origen materno, en quienes se calculó el API, del cual se estableció su diferencia de medias con un error alfa de <0,05. Se determinaron valores máximos de sensibilidad y especificidad, curva ROC e índice de Youden. La t de Student dio un p-valor=0,000 de la diferencia de medias entre las mujeres de tuvieron parto vaginal y las que fueron sometidas a cesárea por DFP de origen materno; el valor del área bajo la curva ROC fue 0,758 (IC 95% 0,695 - 0,814) con un p- valor=0,0001. La máxima sensibilidad (74,42 %. IC 95%: 58,8 % a 86,5 %) y máxima especificidad (73,10 %. IC 95%: 65,8 % a 79,6 %), produjeron un índice de Youden de 0,475 (IC 95% 0,283 - 0,590) el cual está asociado al valor 15,44 (IC 95% 14,19 - 15,83) de la escala del API. El API es una buena herramienta de predicción de mujeres con sospecha de pelvis estrecha y permite su clasificación en tres tipos de pelvis: un valor de API de mas de 15,83 indicaría pelvis aptas para un parto vaginal; un valor de API entre 14,19 y 15,83 se sospecharía de estrechez pélvica; un valor de API menor a 14,19 confirmaría una pelvis estrecha.


Assuntos
Humanos , Feminino , Gravidez , Adolescente , Adulto , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Adulto Jovem , Pelvimetria/métodos , Desproporção Cefalopélvica/diagnóstico , Estudos Transversais , Valor Preditivo dos Testes , Curva ROC , Sensibilidade e Especificidade
2.
Psych J ; 12(6): 838-843, 2023 Dec.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37681239

RESUMO

The present research studied the bidirectional effects of working memory (WM) capacity and emotional regulation; that is, the effect of WM capacity on spontaneous emotional regulation and whether the emotional valence to be regulated has a differential impact on performance in a second WM task. Participants (79) first completed a WM span task (Digit Span), then a self-report emotional intelligence task, which was followed by randomly assigned mock Positive-feedback, Negative-feedback, or No-feedback. In the Negative-feedback and Positive-feedback conditions, a dummy report based on participants' responses to the Trait Meta Mood- 21 was shown on the screen. After that, participants completed another WM task (Running Span). An ordinary least squares multiple linear regression was used to evaluate the predictive power of WM span and experimental condition on post-feedback test performance. The model yielded a significant effect on post-feedback test performance for Negative-feedback and a marginal significant effect for the interaction of this parameter with WM span. The results showed that participants in the Negative-feedback condition performed worse than those assigned to other conditions, and individuals with a higher WM capacity were less susceptible to negative experimental stimuli.


Assuntos
Regulação Emocional , Memória de Curto Prazo , Humanos , Memória de Curto Prazo/fisiologia , Emoções/fisiologia
3.
Int J Psychol ; 58(4): 368-379, 2023 Aug.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36950973

RESUMO

Current research on emotion knowledge and competence emphasises the role of language. Emotion vocabulary is one of the indicators of emotion knowledge that can be objectively measured; however, the metric properties of the scores obtained in tests and tasks to measure it have seldom been adequate. In this study we designed and validated a Spanish emotion vocabulary test (MOVE) employing a corpus approach to construct cloze multiple-choice items, administered the test to a Spanish-speaking sample from two countries, Spain and Argentina, and analysed structural validity of the test items with the Rasch model measurement approach. Eighty-eight items showed adequate fit. Overall, a substantial percentage of variance was explained by a latent variable. Reliability indexes at the test, item, and person level were also adequate. As a vocabulary test, the MOVE can be used in psychological and neurological investigation, as well as in language learning research.


Assuntos
Idioma , Vocabulário , Humanos , Reprodutibilidade dos Testes , Emoções , Espanha , Psicometria
4.
Av. psicol. latinoam ; 41(1): [1-14], ene.-abr. 2023.
Artigo em Espanhol | LILACS | ID: biblio-1512784

RESUMO

El objetivo del presente trabajo consistió en estudiar el rol de la atención selectiva, el componente retentivo y ejecutivo de información de la memoria de trabajo, y la aptitud verbal en la producción de inferencias explicativas en la comprensión de textos expositivos. Con este propósito, 171 estudiantes universitarios completaron pruebas de memoria de trabajo, atención selectiva, aptitud verbal y cuestionarios de inferencias explicativas luego de leer textos expositivos. Los resultados de las ecuaciones estructurales muestran que la producción de inferencias se explica de manera directa por la aptitud verbal y el componente ejecutivo de la memoria de trabajo. El análisis también detectó una relación de interacción de mediación entre las capacidades de producir inferencias explicativas, de atención selectiva y de almacenamiento de información verbal en memoria de trabajo. Estos componentes mostraron un efecto indirecto sobre la capacidad de inferir vía el componente ejecutivo de la memoria de trabajo. Lo anterior sugiere que la capacidad de inferir explicaciones en la comprensión está fuertemente relacionada con la aptitud verbal y la capacidad de procesar información en la memoria de trabajo, mientras que el componente retentivo y de atención selectiva muestran un impacto mediado por la memoria de trabajo.


This research aimed to study the role of selective attention, the retention and executive components of working memory, and verbal aptitude in producing explanatory inferences in the comprehension of expository texts. For this purpose, 171 undergraduates completed tests of working memory, selective attention, verbal aptitude, and explanatory inference questionnaires after reading expository texts. The results of the structural equation modeling indicated that the production of explanatory inferences is determined directly by verbal aptitude and the executive component of verbal working memory. The analysis also detected a mediation interaction effect between the abilities to produce explanatory inferences, selective attention span, and the ability to store verbal information in working memory. These components indirectly affected the ability to infer via the executive component of working memory. This suggest that the ability to make explanatory inferences in the comprehension of expository texts is strongly related to verbal aptitude and the capacity to process information in working memory, while the ability to manage selective attention and retain verbal information in shortterm memory have an impact on the ability to generate inferences mediated by working memory


O objetivo deste trabalho foi estudar o papel da atenção seletiva, do componente retentivo e executivo da memória de trabalho e da aptidão verbal na produção de inferências explicativas na compreensão de textos expositivos. Para tanto, 171 estudantes universitários responderam a testes de memória de trabalho, atenção seletiva, aptidão verbal e questionários de inferência explicativa após a leitura de textos expositivos. Os resultados das equações estruturais mostram que a produção de inferências é diretamente explicada pela aptidão verbal e pelo componente executivo da memória de trabalho. A análise também detectou uma relação de interação mediadora entre a capacidade de produzir inferências explicativas, o tempo de atenção seletiva e a capacidade de armazenar informações verbais na memória de trabalho. Esses componentes mostraram um efeito indireto na capacidade de inferir por meio do componente executivo da memória de trabalho. Isso sugere que a capacidade de inferir explicações na compreensão está fortemente relacionada à aptidão verbal e à capacidade de processar informações na memória de trabalho, enquanto o componente de atenção seletiva e seletiva apresenta impacto, mas mediado pela memória de trabalho.


Assuntos
Humanos
5.
Interdisciplinaria ; 37(1): 31-32, jun. 2020. tab, graf
Artigo em Espanhol | LILACS-Express | LILACS | ID: biblio-1124926

RESUMO

Resumen Comprender un texto es una actividad cognitiva compleja que implica la construcción de una representación mental coherente en la memoria. Un proceso importante para ello es la generación de inferencias. La memoria de trabajo ha mostrado ser un factor cognitivo importante para explicar las diferencias en comprensión e inferencias, al igual que la capacidad de sostener la atención. El propósito del presente trabajo consiste en estudiar la relación entre la memoria de trabajo y la atención sostenida en la comprensión de narraciones en niños de 5 y 6 años. Para ello, se trabajó con una muestra de 100 niños, quienes escucharon tres textos narrativos y respondieron preguntas acerca de su contenido literal e inferencial, y además realizaron dos pruebas de memoria de trabajo y una de atención sostenida. Los resultados indican que los niños de 6 años muestran desempeños mejores y significativos en la comprensión de información literal y en la respuesta a preguntas de inferencia que los niños de 5 años. Además, el análisis de correlación mostró que las medidas de comprensión se hallan vinculadas a medidas de atención sostenida y a medidas de memoria de trabajo. El análisis de senderos sugiere que, en niños de 5 y 6 años, las mejoras logradas en la comprensión general como producto de la edad están mediadas, en parte, por la capacidad del niño para mantener la atención en la narración y almacenar temporalmente la información recibida en la memoria de trabajo mientras la escucha.


Abstract Text comprehension involves the construction of a coherent mental representation, which requires the person to build bridges between the new information and the background knowledge. In adults, establishing associations between information provided by the text is frequently an automatic skill, while for children it implies an important cognitive effort. This ability develops gradually over time and is connected to the generation of inferences. Working memory and the ability to sustain attention are considered two crucial processes for comprehension. The purpose of this study is to analyze the role of working memory and sustained attention in the comprehension of narratives in 5 and 6-year-old children. The study included 100 children of 5 and 6 years of age, of both sexes, that participated with the informed consent of their parents. Three oral texts were narrated to the children by a professional storyteller. For each text, six questions were asked: three of literal content and three of inferential content. Additionally, two working memory tasks were administered with one task of sustained attention. Three analyses were performed: First, a correlation analysis, to study the associations between comprehension, working memory, and sustained attention measures. Then, a comparison analysis of comprehension, working memory, and sustained attention scores between ages 5 and 6. And finally, a path analysis to study the role of age, sustained attention, and working memory on comprehension. Spearman Rho analyses in the whole sample show that literal comprehension had a significant correlation with forward digit span (Rho = .37, p < .001), backward digit span (Rho = .37, p < .001), and with the sustained attention task (Rho = -.37, p < .001). Inferences showed a significant correlation with forward digit span (Rho = .36, p < .001), backward digit span (Rho = .46, p < .001), and with the sustained attention task (Rho = -.37, p < .001). Sustained attention measures showed a significant correlation with forward digit span (Rho = -.34, p < .001) and with backward digit span (Rho = -.37, p < .001). The comparison analysis indicated significant differences between 5- and 6-year-olds in sustained attention measures (t(98) = 3.08, SEM = 5.41, p < .01), literal comprehension (t(98) = 4.05, SEM = 0.59, p < .001), and inferences (U = 750.50, z = 3.50, p < .001), but not in forward digit span (t(98) = 1.43, SEM = 0.34, p = .16) and backward digit span (U = 1043.50, z = 1.49, p = .14). A model of interrelation was proposed with age as independent variable, comprehension as dependent variable, and working memory and sustained attention as mediating variables, being comprehension, a latent factor formed by literal comprehension and inferences, and working memory another latent factor formed by forward digits and backward digits span. The path analysis showed a good fit of the data to the model (c2(5) = 1.93, p = .86; AGFI = .97, CFI = .99, TLI = .99, RMSEA = .00). The analysis showed that 6-year-olds perform better than 5-year-olds in literal and inferential information, and in sustained attention, but not in working memory. The correlation analyses, on the other hand, indicated that comprehension measures are associated to working memory and sustained attention scores and the path analysis indicated that both working memory and sustained attention play a role in comprehension. This suggests that, in 5 and 6-year-olds, age has an effect on the comprehension of general information and the ability to generate inferences, but this effect is mediated, in part, by the child's ability to sustain attention on the narration and to temporarily store the information received while listening to it.

6.
Investig. psicol ; 24(1): 26-34, jun. 2019.
Artigo em Espanhol | LILACS | ID: biblio-1370839

RESUMO

La resolución de problemas aritméticos es una actividad cognitiva compleja, de particular dificultad para niños y niñas, que implica poder identificar los componentes relevantes, las relaciones entre ellos y poder llevar a cabo los cómputos necesarios. Distintos estudios han buscado las variables predictoras de esta capacidad, centrándose en uno de dos aspectos: la competencia matemática del niño/a o su comprensión de texto. El propósito del presente trabajo consistió en estudiar si la incidencia de uno u otro sobre la resolución de problemas en niños y niñas de 4 y 5 años se modifica por la inclusión simultánea de ambos en un modelo de ecuaciones estructurales. A su vez, existe amplia evidencia que sugiere que habilidades de dominio general, como la memoria de trabajo, influyen significativamente sobre la capacidad matemática y de comprensión de texto de los sujetos. Por este motivo, se estudió, adicionalmente, la presencia de un efecto indirecto de la memoria de trabajo verbal y visoespacial y del conocimiento previo sobre la resolución de problemas aritméticos, mediado por las otras dos habilidades. Los resultados obtenidos sugieren que, a esta edad, solo la habilidad matemática tiene un efecto directo significativo sobre la resolución de problemas, y la memoria de trabajo, tanto visual como verbal, un efecto indirecto. Asimismo, se observó que ambos componentes de la memoria de trabajo se vinculan con la capacidad matemática, pero sólo la verbal con la comprensión de texto. Por otro lado, el conocimiento previo sólo parece vincularse con la comprensión


The resolution of arithmetic word problems is a complex cognitive activity which requires the subject to identify the relevant elements, the relationships between them and to be able to carry out the necessary computations. Different studies have searched for predictive variables of this capacity, focusing on one of two aspects: the child's mathematical competence or his or her text comprehension. The purpose of the present work was to study if the incidence of one or the other on the resolution of arithmetic problems in children of 4 and 5 years of age is modified by the simultaneous inclusion of both in a structural equation model (SEM). Additionally, there is ample evidence to suggest that general domain abilities, such as working memory, significantly influence the subject's mathematical and comprehension capacity. That is why we also studied the presence of an indirect effect of verbal and visuospatial working memory, as well as the child's previous knowledge, on the resolution of arithmetic problems, mediated by the other two skills. The results suggest that, at this age, only mathematical ability has a significant direct effect on the resolution of problems, and that working memory, both visual and verbal, have an indirect effect over it. Likewise, both components of working memory are linked with mathematical ability, but only verbal working memory with text comprehension. On the other hand, prior knowledge seems to be associated with text comprehension


Assuntos
Humanos , Masculino , Feminino , Pré-Escolar , Conceitos Matemáticos , Resolução de Problemas , Memória de Curto Prazo
7.
Investig. psicol ; 24(1): 43-49, jun. 2019.
Artigo em Espanhol | LILACS | ID: biblio-1371331

RESUMO

La inteligencia emocional percibida está compuesta por tres factores: la atención a las emociones, la claridad emocional, y la reparación de las emociones (Salovey, Mayer, Goldman, Turvey, & Palfai, 1995). El objetivo de este trabajo es comparar y estudiar las características de la inteligencia emocional percibida en diferentes etapas de la vida. La muestra estuvo compuesta por 456 sujetos, a quienes se les administró el TMMS-21. Se hallaron diferencias en el nivel de atención y recuperación, tanto en la muestra general como en el grupo de mujeres, siendo los adultos quienes reportaron menores índices. Por otro lado, la edad resultó ser un predictor significativo del nivel de atención y reparación. Se explican las diferencias halladas a la luz del desarrollo cognitivo que afectaría la percepción que el sujeto tiene sobre sus habilidades emocionales


Perceived emotional intelligence is composed of three factors: the attention to emotions, emotional clarity and the repair of emotions (Salovey, Mayer, Goldman, Turvey y Palfai, 1995). The purpose of this work is to compare and study the characteristics of perceived emotional intelligence in different stages of life. The sample consisted of 456 subjects, who were administered the TMMS-21. Differences were found in attention and recovery levels, both in the general sample and in the group of women, with adults reporting lower rates. On the other hand, age was a significant predictor of the subject's level of attention and reparation. These differences are explained by the cognitive development that would affect the perception that the subject has about their emotional abilities.


Assuntos
Humanos , Masculino , Feminino , Adolescente , Adulto , Emoções , Cognição , Inteligência Emocional
8.
Eur J Psychol ; 14(3): 632-643, 2018 Aug.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30263075

RESUMO

Emotional intelligence includes self-perception regarding attention to feelings, clarity of feelings and mood repair. The aim of this work is to study the relationship between emotional intelligence, self-concept, and self-esteem. The sample included 137 adolescents from Buenos Aires City, that attended middle school, with a mean age of 13.12 years old (SD = 1.79). Correlation analysis and linear regression analysis were performed. Results showed significant positive correlations between self-esteem and clarity of feelings on the complete sample and the female subsample, and between mood repair and self-esteem on the male subsample. The linear regression analyses showed results on the same line. It´s concluded that positive self-evaluation regarding emotions, emotion comprehension and recovery can minimize the effect of negative experiences.

9.
Investig. psicol ; 23(1): 27-35, abr. 2018.
Artigo em Espanhol | LILACS | ID: biblio-970850

RESUMO

Ciertas habilidades matemáticas básicas que se desarrollan durante la infancia son consideradas nucleares para la posterior adquisición de habilidades más complejas. Conocer su desarrollo puede facilitar la detección de dificultades en niños de forma temprana y la planificación de intervenciones tempranas. Éste estudio se propuso describir el rendimiento de niños de 4, 5 y 6 años en una serie de pruebas destinadas a evaluar estimación y discriminación de cantidades, conteo, subitización, resolución de problemas aritméticos, conocimiento de la serie numérica y comprensión de numerales arábigos. Participaron 206 niños de entre 46 a 83 meses, 112 niñas y 94 niños. Se evaluó el rendimiento de los tres grupos de edad en las tareas de habilidades matemáticas tempranas. Se observaron asociaciones significativas entre las distintas variables (p <0.05), así como diferencias en el rendimiento entre los grupos de edad (F(20, 390) = 14.93; P < .001 ; η = .44).


Certain basic mathematical skills that develop during childhood are considered core numerical abilities for the subsequent acquisition of more complex ones. Knowing their development can facilitate the early detection of difficulties in children and the design of appropriate interventions. This study aimed to describe the performance of children of 4, 5 and 6 years of age in a series of tests designed to assess estimation and discrimination of quantities, counting, subitizing, arithmetic problem solving, knowledge of the counting sequence and understanding of Arabic numerals. These were administered to 206 children, between 46 and 83 months of age, of which 112 were female and 94 male. Results showed significant associations between the different measures (p <0.05), as well as differences in performance between the age groups (F(20, 390) = 14.93, p <.001, η = .44).


Assuntos
Pré-Escolar , Criança , Desenvolvimento Infantil , Conceitos Matemáticos
10.
Subj. procesos cogn ; 22(2): 38-56, 2018.
Artigo em Espanhol | LILACS | ID: biblio-1000338

RESUMO

La inteligencia emocional percibida está compuesta por la percepción sobre la atención a las emociones, la claridad con la que se perciben y la capacidad para recuperación de los estados de ánimo negativo. La necesidad de pertenencia se plantea como una necesidad humana universal y toma particular importancia durante la adolescencia. Objetivo: Analizar la relación entre la inteligencia emocional y la necesidad de pertenencia, estudiando posibles diferencias entre los sexos. Participantes: 399 adolescentes de la Ciudad Autónoma de Buenos Aires, Argentina. Materiales: Se administró la TMMS-21 y la prueba Necesidad de Pertenencia. Resultados: El factor de inteligencia emocional atención a las emociones mostró estar relacionado con la necesidad de pertenencia, en la muestra general. Esta asociación se replica solo en el caso de los varones quizás porque los niveles de atención son, en general, mayores en las mujeres. En el caso de las mujeres se encontró una asociación positiva entre recuperación y miedo al rechazo. Los resultados parecen mostrar una mayor necesidad de pertenencia en varones y un mayor miedo al rechazo en mujeres. (AU)


Perceived emotional intelligence includes a person's attention to emotions, the clarity with which they are perceived and their capacity for recovery of negative moods. Need for belonging is a universal human need and is particularly important during adolescence. Objective: To analyze the relationship between emotional intelligence and need for belonging, studying possible differences between genders. Participants: 399 adolescents in Buenos Aires City, Argentina. Materials: TMMS-21, and the Necessity of Belonging. Results: In the complete sample, attention to emotions was related to need for belonging. This association is replicated only in the case of men, perhaps because the levels of care are, in general, higher in women. In the case of women, a positive association between recovery and fear of rejection was found. The results seem to show a greater need for belonging in men and a greater fear of rejection in women. (AU)


Assuntos
Humanos , Masculino , Feminino , Adolescente , Pesquisa , Inteligência Emocional , Argentina , Psicologia , Psicologia do Adolescente
11.
Liberabit ; 22(2): 197-208, jul.-dic. 2016. tab
Artigo em Espanhol | LILACS | ID: biblio-990135

RESUMO

El objetivo de este trabajo fue comparar los niveles de experiencias óptimas (EO) que los adolescentes reportan en distintas actividades del contexto escolar y extraescolar y analizar cómo se relacionan estos niveles con el autoconcepto y autoestima en la población adolescente de la Ciudad Autónoma de Buenos Aires, Argentina (CABA-AR). Para ello se administró el Inventario Breve de EO y el Perfil de Autopercepción a 399 adolescentes concurrentes a escuelas de educación media de la CABA-AR. Se realizó un ANOVA de medidas repetidas con efecto principal intrasujeto para comparar las EO en cada actividad y un análisis de correlación para estudiar la relación entre EO y el autoconcepto y autoestima. Entre los resultados se encontró que los adolescentes experimentan mayores niveles de ocurrencia de EO en las actividades extracurriculares. Con respecto al autoconcepto y autoestima, las relaciones halladas fueron en todos los casos positivas y mostraron diferencias según el tipo de actividad en el cual se reportaban las EO.


The objective of this study was to compare the levels of optimal experiences (OE) that adolescents report in different activities of the school and out-of-school context and to analyze how these levels are related to self-concept and self- esteem in the adolescent population at Autonomous City of Buenos Aires, Argentina (ACBA-AR). For this purpose, the OE Brief Inventory and the Self-Perception Profile were administered to 399 adolescents attending ACBA-AR high schools. We performed an ANOVA with repeated measures with intrasubject main effect to compare the OEs in each activity and a correlation analysis to study the relationship between OE and self-concept and self-esteem. Among the results, we found that adolescents experience higher levels of occurrence of OE in extracurricular activities. With regard to self-concept and self-esteem, the relationships found were in all cases positive and showed differences according to the type of activity in which OE were reported

12.
Span. j. psychol ; 17: e77.1-e77.7, ene.-dic. 2014. tab, ilus
Artigo em Inglês | IBECS | ID: ibc-130489

RESUMO

Executive Function is a multidimensional construct that includes a wide range of cognitive abilities that allow solving goal-directed behaviors efficiently. Its development begins in early childhood and continues through adolescence. A key aspect of Executive Function is planning, defined as the capacity to generate and organize the necessary step sequence to carry out a goal-directed behavior. The aim of this study was to assess the development of planning in children. The Tower of London task was used in 270 children aged 6, 8, 11, and 13 years. The results showed that the time required to generate and organize the plan to solve a goal-directed problem increases as the difficulty of the problem increases, and that older children need less time to solve problems with a certain level of difficulty than younger children F(15, 1330) = 8.787; MSE = 1.441; p < .01; η2 =.090. These results are in line with the findings that planning develops through childhood and even during the first years of adolescence (AU)


No disponible


Assuntos
Humanos , Masculino , Feminino , Criança , Desenvolvimento da Personalidade , Comportamento Infantil/psicologia , Comportamento do Adolescente/psicologia , Testes Psicológicos/estatística & dados numéricos , Testes Psicológicos/normas , Objetivos , Aptidão , Psicometria/métodos , Psicometria/tendências , Desenvolvimento Infantil/fisiologia , Psicologia da Criança/instrumentação , Psicologia da Criança/métodos , Psicologia da Criança/normas , Educação Infantil/psicologia
13.
Span J Psychol ; 17: E77, 2014 Nov 13.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26054740

RESUMO

Executive Function is a multidimensional construct that includes a wide range of cognitive abilities that allow solving goal-directed behaviors efficiently. Its development begins in early childhood and continues through adolescence. A key aspect of Executive Function is planning, defined as the capacity to generate and organize the necessary step sequence to carry out a goal-directed behavior. The aim of this study was to assess the development of planning in children. The Tower of London task was used in 270 children aged 6, 8, 11, and 13 years. The results showed that the time required to generate and organize the plan to solve a goal-directed problem increases as the difficulty of the problem increases, and that older children need less time to solve problems with a certain level of difficulty than younger children F(15, 1330) = 8.787; MSE = 1.441; p < .01; η2 =.090. These results are in line with the findings that planning develops through childhood and even during the first years of adolescence.


Assuntos
Desenvolvimento Infantil , Função Executiva , Testes Neuropsicológicos , Adolescente , Fatores Etários , Criança , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Resolução de Problemas
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA
...